Jones, 1983

6 USA students per year group (ages 11-16), selected by teachers as average, were interviewed about instances to identify misconceptions.

Learners’ ideas

  • All the students who were interviewed stated that the runner who covered the greater distance was going faster.
  • Some students believe that when one object has caught up to another they must be going at the same speed: "Well for a split second they are at the same point and to get there they both need to go the same speed".
  • Some students considered that speed and velocity were synonymous.
  • Velocity is different from speed but the differences are not accepted scientific ones: "Velocity can be slowing down but speed, it is normally rising, but velocity can go both ways."
  • Velocity is the same as the acceleration.
  • Students realised that acceleration involved the idea of a change in speed but the relationship between the two quantities was not the scientific definition (i.e. change in speed/change in time). All students assumed increasing speed always meant increasing acceleration.
  • Some students indicated that if an object was going fast it also must have had acceleration.

Further suggestions

  • One object passing another dominates some students' ideas of relative speeds.
  • One senior physics class were asked if they considered an object falling or rolling down a slope to have increasing acceleration. Despite the fact that these students had just been taught the theoretical aspects of acceleration, 70% answered that it would.

The study

Aims

Identify concepts of speed, velocity and acceleration held by students.

How the evidence was collected

Interviews were recorded and transcribed: Students were shown drawings on cards which represented various instances and events e.g. car slowing down, speeding up, overtaking, a ball dropping from an aircraft, throwing the ball up, rolling a ball down an incline and spinning a ball around on the end of a string. The interviews were conducted in a standard questioning format, but at any point, the interviewer could choose to probe more deeply into a student's understanding by extending the discussion. In addition, to verify some aspects of the interview work, a multiple-choice survey was also administered.

Details of the sample

30 students, 6 per year group, from five different schools in the USA, consisting of three intermediate (11-13 years) and two secondary (13-16 years) were interviewed. Teachers were asked to send pupils of average ability.

For the follow-up multiple-choice surveys: 113 13-14 years, 110 Form 14-15 years, 96 Form 15-16 years, 52 16-17 years (including 56 non-physics students).

Limit Less Campaign

Support our manifesto for change

The IOP wants to support young people to fulfil their potential by doing physics. Please sign the manifesto today so that we can show our politicians there is widespread support for improving equity and inclusion across the education sector.

Sign today